Stay Informed
Follow us on social media accounts to stay up to date with REHVA actualities
This article was first published in the
ASHRAE Journal March 2013, pp 46-50.
It is republished with the permission of the authors and ASHRAE Journal
David P. Wyon and PawelWargockiInternational Centre for Indoor Environment
and Energy DTU Civil Engineering, Technical University of Denmark. dpw@byg.dtu.dk |
Engineers
are used to having to act on incomplete evidence, but if they are wise they
like to have this evidence reviewed for them by specialists in any field that
is outside their own experience and training. As experienced researchers in
this particular field, we are often asked to give our best estimate of how and
to what extent performance is affected by different aspects of indoor climate,
so we now offer this very brief summary of our personal opinions, in the form
of answers to 40 frequently asked questions (FAQs).
Our answers are based on the results of the behavioral
experiments that have been conducted to date. We offer no opinions on the
long-term health effects of indoor environmental quality. We provide some
references to where the relevant findings and a discussion of them may be
found, but there is not enough space for all such references. We also list some
questions we cannot answer as topics for future research in this area.
There are four main reasons:
1) It
is the added value of occupant performance that pays for indoor environmental
quality (Fisk et al., 2011);
2) Performance is affected in the short-term by the combined effects of all indoor environmental factors, while subjective and physiological responses are usually selected because they are a function of one specific factor;
3) It
turns out that thermal and air quality effects on performance can be observed
even when there are no observable effects on comfort or on health-related symptom
intensity (Wargocki et al.; 2004; Wyon,
2004; Wargocki and Wyon,
2006); and
4) The
primary purpose of factory, office and school buildings is to provide an
optimal indoor environment for work and for learning to work.
We have found that they usually reduce the rate of working, with little or no effect on accuracy (Wyon, 2004; Wargocki and Wyon, 2006).
In our experience, because people tend to reduce their rate of work until they are again able to achieve an acceptable error rate.
In general, tasks that require concentration (clear thinking and symbolic manipulation), memory and original thought (Wyon, 2004; Tham and Willem, 2005; Lan et al., 2011).
Most mental work involves concentration and is thus likely to be similarly affected.
Excessive concentration can impair recognition memory and creative thinking, so as moderate warmth leads to lowered arousal it can paradoxically improve the performance of work that includes such tasks.
Not always, although levels below 15% RH were found to impair visual acuity and the performance of tasks requiring continuous acquisition of visual data, which are both crucial in process control, driving, piloting an airplane and work with PCs (Wyon et al., 2006).
Raised temperatures have been found to increase end-tidal CO2 (ETCO2 is an indicator of mild “acidosis”, which is an increase in the concentration of CO2 in the blood) and to decrease oxygen saturation in blood (SpO2), both of which are likely to be detrimental for mental work (Lan et al., 2011).
Poor air quality may lead to mild acidosis, exactly as raised temperature does, because it has been found to reduce CO2 emission from occupants (Bako-Biro et al., 2005). If so, this may be why both factors have such similar effects. Satish et al. (2012) have recently shown that increasing the ambient CO2 concentration artificially can decrease performance, suggesting that ambient CO2 may have to be regarded as a pollutant instead of as an indicator of low outdoor air supply rate.
For adults, up to 5% in the laboratory (Wyon, 2004), up to 10% in the field (Wargocki et al., 2004).For schoolchildren, over 20% (Wargocki and Wyon, 2006).
It would seem so, as driver vigilance was found to be reduced by up to 30% by warmth in field intervention experiments lasting only 1 h (Wyon et al., 1996).
As staff costs per unit of floor area exceed operating costs by 100:1, the effects observed are seldom negligible (Fisk et al., 2011).
We have found that their performance is more affected, not less, and believe that this is because children in school are by definition doing work that is new to them, while adult workers are usually very familiar with the work they do and thus are better able to cope with environmental effects that make their work more difficult.
We believe not, as most workers in modern factories have to interact with computers, just as office workers do.
Many field
studies have found that the negative effects of poor working conditions
are greater in real workplaces than would have been predicted from laboratory
experiments (Wargocki et al., 2004; Tham and Willem, 2005). This may be because laboratory experiments use paid subjects, who tend to exert
more effort than they would routinely in the course of a necessarily brief
laboratory exposure to poor IEQ.
If subjects are highly motivated they can sometimes maintain performance during short exposures to poor indoor environmental quality. Negative effects on fatigue may then be found instead. Additionally, some studies may simply have missed the subtle changes in performance that are caused by slightly sub-optimal indoor environmental conditions.
Logically, yes, and, although environmental effects on component skills have yet to be validated as predictors of overall productivity, call-centre results use “bottom-line” measures of the call volume achieved in practice (Wargocki et al., 2004; Tham and Willem, 2005), and schoolwork is what children do in school (Wargocki and Wyon, 2006; Haverinen-Shaughnessy et al., 2010; Bako-Biro et al., 2012).
Yes. Field intervention experiments examine directly what does happen in practice, often over periods of several weeks or even months. Tests of year-end educational attainment have been found to support predictions based on short-term intervention experiments in classrooms (Wargocki and Wyon, 2006; Haverinen-Shaughnessy et al., 2010).
It depends on the length of exposure. Most continuous work is in fact performed in periods lasting less than 5 hours, followed by a break, and even laboratory experiments may include 5-hour exposures.
Not proven. But surely schoolwork is assumed by teachers to promote learning? Test scores used by teachers and regulators to observe progress in learning have been found to correlate with spot measurements of ventilation (Haverinen-Shaughnessy et al., 2010).
Yes. Very similar results were obtained when the same experiments were repeated in Singapore (Tham and Willem, 2005).
Until we know which pollutants are causing the negative effects on people, the outdoor air supply rate per person seems to be the most reliable indicator (Seppänen et al., 2006).
It has been experimentally shown that it does (Wyon, 2004), except in the case of pollutants with no odor.
No. Sensory habituation ensures that increasingly poor air quality may be underestimated, except by visitors (Wyon, 2004).
So far there is no reliable evidence that they can. Self-estimated productivity may simply indicate the effort they are aware of exerting (Wyon, 2004), and/or wishful thinking and a desire to placate management.
Air temperature is not a reliable indicator
in any absolute sense, because performance is a function of the heat balance of
the body (which is affected by clothing, metabolic rate, air velocity, etc.),
but in a given work situation it is a very useful basis for comparison. In the
cold, manual dexterity is progressively impaired as the body actively reduces
finger temperature to conserve heat, and in slightly warm conditions, mental
performance has been found to decrease when finger temperatures approach their
maximum value of about 36C and sweating must be initiated to maintain the
body’s heat balance. Finger temperatures in the 30-34C range are therefore a
reliable indicator that thermal conditions are optimal for most kinds of
performance.
Not always, because they may be able to avoid discomfort by working less. This implies that the adaptive model of thermal comfort should NOT be used in isolation to justify energy conservation measures, because that could lead to conditions that cause sub-optimal performance and productivity (Lan et al., 2011).
Yes, in theory, because they do co-vary. But the data is still too meager to create a robust relationship (Tham and Willem, 2005).
Poor ventilation does increase absenteeism (Milton et al, 2000), but so do many other factors.
Generations of experienced teachers ensured that children spent brief but regular periods in fresher air, i.e. outdoors, even in cold weather. Although this strategy does not seem to have been validated experimentally, our view is that it might work just as well for adults as for children.
No, because they will not be opened spontaneously unless it is also warm and because opening windows will often be seen as a waste of heating or cooling energy.
Yes, to the extent that users are aware that ambient conditions are sub-optimal.
No. It can even have the reverse effect if it passes through particulate filters that are full of dust (Wargocki et al., 2004).
There is no evidence that it does, even though dust is expected to have negative effects on chronic health problems. Short-term effects of poor air quality on the performance of school work remained after airborne dust had been removed, so the negative effects observed were attributed to gas-phase air pollutants (Wargocki et al., 2008).
No, because negative effects on performance will increase progressively, even if some subjective habituation takes place (Kolarik et al., 2009).
No. They are a function of the heat balance of the body.
Anything that increases heat loss from the body makes raised air temperature more tolerable.
Physiological acclimatization to heat requires hard physical exertion well beyond what is necessary for the performance of office work.
We have identified the following 10 high priority research topics:
1) Are
the combined effects of temperature and indoor air quality additive?
2) How
does performance vary with self-estimated performance?
3) Which
component skills are affected by indoor temperature and air quality effects?
4) Is
high-level work involving decision-making and creative thinking similarly
affected?
5) Are
leisure activities negatively affected by poor indoor environmental quality?
6) Is
sleep affected by temperature and IAQ and if so does this affect next-day
performance?
7) What
is the economic impact of all these effects on different kinds of productivity?
8) What
is the most cost-effective way to reduce the negative effects of poor IEQ?
9) How
can energy be conserved without affecting performance?
10)How
do energy certification schemes affect productivity?
We believe that that the following 4 topics should be addressed by future research:
1) Do
thermal and indoor air quality effects on acidosis decrease performance?
2) Is
the acidosis caused by shallow breathing or by decreased gas exchange in the
lungs?
3) Which
gas-phase indoor air pollutants have this effect, and can it be prevented?
4) Are
any other mechanisms involved?
The preparation of this paper was supported by the research funds of the International Centre for Indoor Environment and Energy.
Bakó-Biró, Z.S., Wargocki, P., Wyon,
D.P. and Fanger, P.O. (2005) “Poor indoor air quality
slows down metabolic rate of office workers”, In Proceedings of Indoor Air
2005, Beijing, Vol. I(1), 76-80.
Bakó-Biró, Z., Clements-Croome, D.J., Kochhar, N Awbi, H.B. and
Williams, M.J. (2012) “Ventilation rates in schools and pupils’ performance”,
Building and Environment, 48, 215-223.
Fisk, W.
J., Black, D. and Brunner, G. (2011) “Benefits and costs of improved IEQ in
U.S. offices”, Indoor Air, 21, 357–367.
Haverinen-Shaughnessy, U., Moschandreas, D. and Shaughnessy,
R.J. (2010) “Association between substandard classroom ventilation rates and
students’ academic achievement”, Indoor Air, 21, 121-131.
Kolarik,
J., Toftum, J., Olesen,
B.W. and Shitzer, A. (2009) “Occupant response and
office work performance in environment with moderately drifting operative
temperatures (RP-1269)”, HVAC&R Research, 15(5), 931-960. (from ASHRAE
1269-TRP)
Lan, L., Wargocki,
P., Wyon, D.P. and Liam, Z. (2011) “Effects of thermal
discomfort in an office on perceived air quality, SBS symptoms, physiological
responses and human performance”, Indoor Air, 21, 376-390.
Milton,
D.K., Glencross, P.M. and Walters, M.D. (2000) “Risk
of Sick Leave Associated with Outdoor Air Supply Rate, Humidification, and
Occupant Complaints”, Indoor Air, 10, 212–221.
Satish
U., Mendell MJ., Shekhar K,
Hotchi T., Sullivan D., Streufert
S. and Fisk WJ (2012) Is CO2 an Indoor Pollutant? Direct Effects of
Low-to-Moderate CO2 Concentrations on Human Decision-Making Performance.
Environ Health Perspect. doi:10.1289/ehp.1104789
Seppänen,
O. Fisk, W. and Lei, Q.H. (2006) “Ventilation and performance in office work”,
Indoor Air, vol 16, 28-35
Tham,
K.W. and Willem, H.C (2005) “Temperature and ventilation effects on performance
and neurobehavioral symptoms of tropically acclimatized call center operators near thermal neutrality”. ASHRAE
Transactions, vol. 111 (2), DE-05-10-7
Wargocki,
P., Wyon, D.P. and Fanger,
P.O. (2004) “The performance and subjective responses of call-centre operators
with new and used supply air filters at two outdoor air supply rates”, Indoor
Air, 14 (Suppl. 8), 7-16.
Wargocki,
P. and Wyon, D.P. (2006) ”Effects
of HVAC on Student Performance”, ASHRAE Journal, October, 22-28. (from ASHRAE
1257-TRP)
Wargocki
P., Wyon, D.P., Jensen, K. and Bornehag,
C.G. (2008) “The effects of electrostatic filtration and supply air filter
condition in classrooms on the performance of schoolwork by children”,
HVAC&R Research, Vol. 14, No. 3, pp. 327-344. (from ASHRAE 1257-TRP)
Wyon,
D.P. (2004) “The effects of indoor air quality on performance and
productivity”, Indoor Air, 14, Supplement 7, 92-101
Wyon,
D.P., Wyon, I. and Norin,
F. (1996) “Effects of moderate heat stress on driver vigilance in a moving
vehicle”, Ergonomics, 39, 61-75
Wyon,D.P., Fang,L., Lagercrantz,L. and Fanger, P.O.
(2006) “Experimental determination of the limiting criteria for human exposure
to low winter humidity indoors (RP-1160)”, HVAC&R Research, 12, 201-213.
(from ASHRAE 1160-TRP)
Follow us on social media accounts to stay up to date with REHVA actualities
0